Contact
Tom Novack, Ph.D.
University of Alabama at Birmingham
Citation Novack, T. (2004). The
Cognitive Log. The Center for Outcome Measurement in Brain
Injury. http://www.tbims.org/
combi/coglog ( accessed
).
Cog-Log
Properties
Normative
Sample: The Cog-Log was presented to 83 individuals without
acquired brain injury (Alderson & Novack, 2003). Individuals
without known neurological insult received average total Cog-Log
scores of 28 (± 2), and mean individual item scores were
greater than or equal to 2.4. Demographic variables of age, education,
and gender did not predict total or individual item scores (p>.05).
Stepwise discriminant analyses on a sample of 82 brain injured patients
and 82 normal controls matched for age, education, and gender revealed
that a cut-off score of 25 correctly classified 88.4% of individuals
in their respective groups.
Reliability:
A sample of 150 individuals with acquired brain injury were examined
with the Cog-Log. Most of the sample (80%) had sustained moderate
to severe TBI, with the remainder including patients with CVA and
anoxia. For detailed information about this sample and the properties
of the scale please read Alderson & Novack (2003). Internal
consistency analysis (Cronbach’s alpha) was conducted for
the total Cog-Log score. Inter-rater reliability estimates (Spearman’s
rho) were calculated using a subset of 19 patients (75 total observations),
who did not differ in age, education, injury type, or injury severity
(p > .05) from the sample as a whole. Interrater reliability
coefficients for each of the 10 Cog-Log items ranged from .749 (Go/No-go
task) to 1.0 (Time Estimation). Standard errors of measurement are
no more than .10 for single item scores, which range from 0 to 3.
The
overall internal consistency of the 10-item Cog-Log is .778, with
a standard error of measurement of .53. Factor analysis of the Cog-Log
using principal components extraction (N = 150) revealed a unitary
factor (Eigenvalue = 3.48). Although all scale items loaded on this
factor, those items involving delayed recall of verbal information
and recitation of months backwards demonstrated the highest loadings,
suggesting a stronger contribution of complex working memory and
long-term recall to this unitary factor.
Concurrent
Validity: Multiple regression evaluation of individual
Cog-Log items revealed a number of significant relationships to
well-established standardized neuropsychological measures of verbal
memory, attention/working memory, and executive functions assessed
on the same day the Cog-Log was administered (Alderson & Novack,
2003). In order to account for shared variance among Cog-Log items,
a stepwise multiple regression evaluation was utilized. No a priori
hypotheses were made about the entry order of Cog-Log items. Age,
education, and estimated IQ were also entered as independent factors
to account for individual differences that are also known to affect
raw score performance on standardized cognitive measures. Immediate
story recall (WMS-R) was significantly predicted by the Cog-Log
item assessing immediate address recall (p<.001). Delayed story
recall (WMS-R) was predicted by Cog-Log items assessing delayed
address recall (p<.001) and months reversed (p=.003). Cog-Log
items assessing immediate and delayed address recall, orientation
to date, and reversal of months were significantly associated with
performance on standardized list learning measures. Performance
on the initial list learning trial of the RAVLT was significantly
predicted by Cog-Log immediate address recall (p=.004). Cog-Log
items assessing delayed address recall (p<.001) and orientation
to date (p=.001) predicted performance on the final RAVLT learning
trial. Delayed address recall also predicted delayed recall of RAVLT
list items (p<.001). Cog-Log items assessing immediate address
recall (p<.001) and time estimation (p=.003) predicted forward
digit span (WAIS-R), while Cog-Log months backward (p<.001) predicted
backwards digit span (WAIS-R). Finally, performance on part B of
the Reitan Trail Making Test (TMTB) was predicted by the Cog-Log
item months backward (p<.001).
Predicting Outcome: In a sample of 50 individuals
participating in acute rehabilitation following moderate to severe
TBI, the lowest Cog-Log score during rehabilitation was found to
predict cognitive outcome at one year after injury (Lee, LeGalbo,
Baños, & Novack, 2004). The Cog-Log was found to contribute
significantly to the prediction of outcome in three of six neuropsychological
domains (attention, executive functioning, and visuomotor-visuospatial
abilities) after controlling for demographics and injury severity.
The companion measure to the Cog-Log, the Orientation Log, was found
to predict memory functioning at one year post-injury. The neuropsychological
tests employed were those being used by the TBI Model System program.
The results underscore the potential usefulness of using both the
O-Log and Cog-Log. The results of the study do not suggest that
the Cog-Log can be considered a substitute for a neuropsychological
examination. Rather, the study confirms that the Cog-Log is providing
a general measure of cognitive ability.